Skip to main content

Scientists’ views about lay perceptions of volcanic hazard and risk

Abstract

We present data from a survey of scientists from volcano observatories and monitoring institutions around the world. The scientists were asked about the hazards from the volcanoes that they work on, their perception of the likely magnitude and impacts of eruptions, and their views about local people’s awareness of the risk. They were also asked about how well different groups are trusted by local people, and about their views concerning the need to warn people about changes in the volcanic risk. We show that scientists were generally concerned about risk from the volcanoes that they worked on, and also that many scientists felt that their own view of the risk was different from that of locals. Perceived trust in scientists depended upon both social factors and volcanic risk. We discuss the implications of these results for precautionary decision-making on active volcanoes.

Background

Volcanic crises involve high levels of uncertainty and high stakes. Volcanic eruptions are unusual among natural hazards because of their wide range of hazards and attendant impacts. Major eruptions are infrequent, making them a forgotten threat. This is compounded by the potential length of volcanic crises – eruptions may last for decades, as has occurred on Montserrat (Wadge et al. [2014]), and require continual scientific advice for policymakers. Thus, many nations have volcano observatories, with specific mandates to monitor volcanoes and provide scientific advice when needed. At a time when the human population has exceeded 7 billion, and perhaps as many as 1 billion live within 100 km of an active volcano (Donovan and Oppenheimer [2014]), the global risk from volcanoes is high. However, the low probability of eruptions at individual volcanoes over short, human timescales makes communication of risk difficult prior to major volcanic emergencies. Furthermore, populations in the vicinity of active volcanoes vary in their perception of risk (e.g. Gregg et al. [2004], [2008]; Dominey-Howes and Minos-Minopoulos [2004]; Haynes et al. [2007], [2008]; Bird et al. [2009], [2010]; Paton et al. [2008], [2010]). Volcanologists working at volcano observatories thus have an important role in both the assessment and the communication of risk. This involves a judgement about how to frame scientific reports and how to communicate most effectively. In part, scientists’ judgements about the risk perception of the population may affect the ways in which information is framed for policymakers and the public. This is a significant issue for disaster risk reduction for volcanic risk, since scientific advice is important in the assessment of volcanic activity, and stakes are high. This paper explores this using quantitative survey data.

Disaster risk reduction (DRR) requires an integrated approach from the social and natural sciences. Hewitt ([1983]) reinforced the observation of White ([1945]) that natural hazards become disasters primarily due to social factors rather than physical ones. This has resulted in a bifurcation in the literature, with scientists focussing on better characterising hazards and social scientists focussing on vulnerability reduction. A problem with this schism is that the social role of scientists and of social scientists themselves is neglected in the literature. Risk is generally conceptualised as a combination of the likelihood of an event and its consequences. In DRR, risk is generally presented as a function of both hazard and vulnerability (e.g. Wisner et al. [2004]), but recent DRR research has tended to focus on the latter (e.g. Hewitt [1983], [2000], [2013]; Cutter [1996]; Bankoff [2001]; Gaillard [2010]) – and within this, physical, biological and social vulnerability (e.g. Brooks [2003]). Bankoff ([2001]) puts forward the view that vulnerability is itself influenced by its origin as a Western concept and by the assumption that a solution is the provision of Western expertise. This argument has led to a body of literature that seeks to incorporate in DRR both “global” scientific knowledge and local, indigenous knowledge (such as traditional methods of coping with hazards), for example Mercer et al. ([2010]) and Gaillard and Mercer ([2013]). However, the importance of local knowledge notwithstanding, in the early stages of a volcanic crisis, there may be heavy dependence on scientific advice and risk assessment in order to assess the likelihood of an eruption, and the views of scientists about the way that populations might be affected by hazards are important in the advisory process. Furthermore, scientists may be asked to communicate their assessment to the policymakers and to the public (e.g. Haynes et al. [2008]). The ways in which scientists perceive any differences between their own view of the risk and that of the population or the policy makers is likely to affect the communication process.

Risk perception is not a simple matter of knowledge, but is affected by the presence of other risks (such as going hungry or anxiety about employment; e.g. Gaillard [2008]; Wachinger et al. [2013]). The relative importance of different risks to individuals may influence the behaviour of populations more than the likelihood of the events causing risk or their potential outcomes (e.g. Slovic [2000]; Pidgeon et al. [2003]; Pidgeon and Fischoff, [2011]; Gaillard [2008]). Furthermore, the trust of populations in different sources of information can affect volcanic risk perception (e.g. Haynes et al. [2007], [2008]). Trust is critically important in risk communication, particularly when stakes are high: poorly trusted information is unlikely to be acted upon. Trust can be affected by diverse factors, including perceived financial gain or loss from the risk (e.g. Eiser et al. [2007], [2008]); past experience (e.g. Frewer et al. [2003]); conflicting messages (Haynes et al. [2007]); perceived political motivation (Poortinga et al. [2004]). In general, trust is linked to perceived competence, openness and consistency (Renn and Levine [1991]; Poortinga and Pidgeon [2003]).

Scientists have been identified as a well trusted group by numerous studies (e.g. Haynes et al. [2008]; Eiser et al. [2008]), and are increasingly expected to take part in the risk communication process. Yet the L’Aquila trial in Italy and the conviction of scientists in 2012 for “having given out falsely reassuring information to members of the public” suggests that involvement of scientists in risk communication can be problematic (Alexander [2014]). Related to this, there is a large literature concerning the effectiveness of warnings (e.g. reviews in Mileti and Sorensen [1990]; Dash and Gladwin [2007]). This suggests that warning is a social process, and its impact will be defined by those issuing the warning, those receiving it, and the characteristics of the message itself (Mileti and O'Brien [1992]). However, much of this literature has focussed on the ways in which warnings have been received by populations, rather than the factors that might affect the composition of a warning message itself.

In volcanology, past crises have demonstrated the complexity of managing low-probability, high-impact risks, in light of high levels of uncertainty combined with high stakes. The 1976 non-eruption of La Soufriere de Guadeloupe is an example of this. In this case, 73,000 people were evacuated for nine months as a result of volcanic activity, but no eruption occurred. Thus, with hindsight, the evacuation was perceived by some as unnecessary. Hincks et al. ([2014]) have showed using belief-based probabilistic methods that the decision to evacuate was appropriate given the high levels of uncertainty, but the event was still viewed by some as a “false alarm” or failure by some volcanologists at the time (e.g. Tazieff [1977]). This in turn caused concern about the erosion of trust in scientists and the potential for future public resistance to scientific caution. Anxiety about the likely reaction of the public to a warning is likely to affect the decision of when warnings should be issued. Indeed, anxiety about perceived anomalies in lay risk perception may also influence the communication process, particularly if there is a fear of accusations about false alarms.

There have been several studies of lay and expert perceptions of risk (e.g. Hansen et al. [2003]; Fischhoff et al. [1982]; Slovic [1999]; Slovic et al. [2004]; Bostrom [1997]; Wynne [1996]). Differences between lay and expert opinions concerning biotechnology and genetically modified crops have been demonstrated, for example (e.g. Savadori et al. [2004]; Wynne [2002]). The hypothesis that lay people perceive risk differently to experts due to a lack of knowledge (the “knowledge deficit” model) has been challenged by increasing awareness of the complexity of lay risk perception (e.g. Hansen et al. [2003]; Wachinger et al. [2013]) and by the observation that experts may also be vulnerable to a range of heuristics and biases (e.g. Bostrom [1997]; Eiser [2004]; Rowe and Wright [2001]; Slovic [2000]; Sjoberg [2000]; Tversky and Kahneman [1973], [1974]; Kahneman et al. [1982]). Scientists may have access to additional knowledge, but their engagement with their subject on a daily basis could affect their judgement of the risk, especially if there is no clear and robust method for quantifying that risk (Tversky and Kahneman [1973]). Scientific judgement concerning lay perception of risk may also be important in the scientific advisory process that feeds into policy; for example, Hansen et al. ([2003]) report that experts view public concern about food risk as excessive (see also Eiser et al. [2002]). This raises questions for politicians seeking to reconcile lay and expert opinions under uncertainty (e.g. Krebs [2011]). It is therefore important for those responsible for managing volcanic emergencies to understand both the risk perception within the scientific community, and also its views about public perception of risks. In terms of disaster risk reduction, a best practice approach might involve public participatory activities in order to facilitate the integration of scientific and local knowledge (Gaillard and Mercer [2013]; Wisner et al. [2012]). Scientists’ perceptions of the public response will be relevant and this is the topic discussed in this paper.

In this paper, we present data from a survey of 95 volcanologists from 25 countries concerning the risk from the volcanoes with which they are most familiar and local people’s perceptions of that risk. Scientists were initially asked about the types of hazard that could ensue from a “major eruption” of the volcano, and the magnitude of these hazards. They were also asked about the likelihood of major eruptions and the potential for temporary and permanent evacuations of populations as a result. They were then asked about their views on false alarms and precaution. Finally, scientists were asked their opinions about how well they thought that local people understand the risks from the hazards and how well different groups were trusted. We draw some conclusions from the results to inform disaster risk reduction practice around active volcanoes.

Methods

Purpose

The purpose of the study was to assess how well volcano observatory scientists think that the population around the volcano that they work on perceive the risk from that volcano, and whether this affects the scientists’ own views about precautionary decision-making. Since risk communication is a two-way process, scientists’ perceptions of local people are likely to affect the ways in which they frame risk communication.

Survey design

The survey was targeted at volcanologists working at volcano-monitoring institutions. It was disseminated online, using the “Volcano Listserv” facilitated by Arizona State University and also via the World Organisation of Volcano Observatories. Scientists who had experience at volcano observatories were asked to take part and were directed to the survey. It initially asked them to specify a volcano that they work on or have worked on, and answer the questions with regard to that volcano. This was necessary for comparability because some volcano observatories are responsible for many volcanoes. Furthermore, they were asked to conceptualise a “major eruption” of the volcano and the hazards that would ensue from such an eruption, acknowledging that this scenario would differ between individuals.

The survey had three main sections – a demographic section, a section about scientists’ own views and a section concerning scientists’ views about local perceptions. It was available in English, French, Italian and Spanish. The demographic section asked respondents to specify their citizenships, age, gender, how long they had been in their current profession, their role in the organisation (e.g. scientist, director, technician), their discipline and highest level of education. They were also asked how rewarding they find their profession, how close to the volcano they live, how long they had lived in their community and whether or not they were emotionally attached to that community. The scale used for these questions was a seven-point Likert scale (Likert [1932]), with specified end points; for example, the scale for the final question was “definitely yes” and the other “definitely no”. Other points on the scale were not labelled to allow the respondent to select their position relative to the two end points. This type of scale was used for all of the scale variables in the survey.

Part I of the survey asked scientists for their own views.

Hazards and likelihoods

This section asked scientists to specify the hazards they would expect from their chosen volcano in a major eruption, and then to provide the most likely length and maximum likely length of any hazardous flows (lahars, debris avalanches, pyroclastic flows (PFs) and blasts). In essence they were being asked to specify a likely scenario in the event of any eruption, and which would therefore influence their advice to decision makers and the public in the case of volcanic unrest. They were then asked the likelihood of an eruption within 3, 30 and 300 years – both on a scale and as a “1 in N” frequency – and the number of years in which they felt there would “certainly” be an eruption, and the number in which there was a 50–50 chance of an eruption. They were also asked how confident they were about these answers.

Activity, forecasting and understanding

The scientists were asked questions about how much the volcanic activity changes from week to week and year to year (“not at all” to “a great deal”); how easy it is to forecast these changes and how easy it is to understand them (“extremely easy” to “extremely difficult”). These variables were defined as “change”, “forecast” and “understand” respectively. In the discussion, the “forecast” and “understand” variables are used as measures of the level of uncertainty that was perceived by scientists to pervade their work.

Impacts and plans

Scientists were asked about the likely damage to property, likely loss of life without evacuation (“extremely unlikely” to “extremely likely”), need to evacuate temporarily or permanently (“not at all necessary” to “essential”), and the effectiveness of current warning plans and current evacuation plans (“extremely ineffective” to “extremely effective”). These were defined as the “impact” variables.

Precaution and false alarms

Scientists were asked to show their level of agreement with four statements concerning precaution and false alarms (’strongly disagree’ to ’strongly agree’):

  1. 1.

    If too many warnings are given, people stop taking them seriously.

  2. 2.

    It’s always better to be safe than sorry.

  3. 3.

    If one thinks there’s any risk at all, the public must be warned.

  4. 4.

    Warnings that turn out to be unnecessary do more harm than good.

Part II repeated the questions on likelihoods, activity, forecasting and impacts, but asked for scientists’ views about the perceptions of local people. For example, “how much do local people, on average, think that the activity at [volcano name] changes from week to week?” A further question about responses was added, asking how effective local people thought that they themselves would be if they had to improvise a response to an eruption (“extremely ineffective” to “extremely effective”).

Trust

Scientists were asked to rate local people’s trust in the following: scientists, national governments, local governments, family and friends, local community leaders (e.g. priests, elders), and the news media (“do not trust at all” to “trust completely”).

Surveys of any population require careful design, bearing in mind the potential for sample biases. For this survey, we focussed on volcano observatory scientists and contacted them via volcanology email list, which would limit the scope of the survey to countries where use of email is widespread. We have no respondents from some countries with many volcanoes (such as Indonesia), and large numbers from Anglophone countries. There are thus some inevitable limitations on the data. There is debate in the literature concerning the placement of demographic questions (e.g. Colton and Covert [2007]; Green et al. [2000]). For example, Teclaw et al. ([2012]) found that respondents were less likely to respond to demographic questions at the end of surveys (which is the conventional placement), and that placing them at the start of the survey does not impact response rate. Since the demographic questions were important in understanding the results and also in setting up the survey – the respondents had to choose a volcano that they worked on – we placed these questions at the start of the survey.

Statistical methods

The sets of variables for scientists’ own views were averaged as follows:

  • The three likelihood values, alpha = 0.88

  • The two changeability variables, alpha = 0.84

  • The two understanding variables, alpha = 0.86

  • The two forecasting variables, alpha = 0.75

  • The four impact variables, alpha = 0.87

  • All of the trust variables, alpha = 0.71

  • The first three trust variables were also divided into another variable: (scientists, local authorities and national government), alpha = 0.81

In addition to these variables, predictor variables were computed from the demographic data. In the context of this study a predictor variable is a variable that can be used to try to predict other variables. For example, citizenships, job description, discipline, education, gender, language and volcano were all coded and used in statistical tests to look for relationships between these variables and the other responses to the survey. Volcanoes were coded by region, tectonic setting, recent activity (in last 5, 50, 100 years), and whether they were in developed or less developed countries. Citizenships were also coded by region. A “precaution” variable was calculated by taking unweighted means of statements 2 and 3 (r = 0.42, p = 0.00). However, the other two statements, though they both concern false alarms, were less well correlated and are taken individually below. A scale variable, years since last eruption” was also computed; however, for six of the volcanoes, the date of the last eruption is not known. All the demographic variables were assessed for multicollinearity (cross-correlation), and where this existed, care was taken to ensure that it did not prejudice results.

An analysis of frequency data established that the majority of variables were non-parametric in distribution (failed Levene’s test for homogeneity of variance, and the Shapiro-Wilk test for normality). Therefore, non-parametric tests were applied to test for the influence of particular predictor variables.

The Kruskal-Wallis analysis of variance (ANOVA) procedure (H) was used to test for significant relationships where the predictor variable was discontinuous and had more than two categories (e.g. “director”, ’scientist”, “technician”). This test ranks the medians of the results for each category of the predictor, and is appropriate for non-parametric data. Significance was taken at the 5% level. Similar to this, the Mann–Whitney test (U) was used for predictor variables with two categories. Wilcoxon’s signed rank test (T) for related samples was used to compare the scores between scientists’ own views and those of locals. This test ranks the differences between two variables (scientists’ views versus their views about locals) and assesses the likelihood of these differences occurring by chance. Relationships between scale variables were assessed using Spearman’s ρ, a non-parametric correlation coefficient, and where appropriate, multiple regression analyses were carried out using a stepwise method in which variables are either added or removed from the analysis until only variables with significant values are included in the model. For variables with a scientists’ view and a perceived local view, a hierarchical regression was used, to account for variance that was the result of anchoring to the scientists’ own views. For the same reason, partial correlations were applied to these variables. Partial correlations are denoted by “r” and Spearman’s correlations are denoted by ρ. Means of variables are demoted “M”; standard deviations are denoted ’sD’.

Results

In this section, we present the results of the survey in three stages. Initially, we describe the demographic patterns within the data, which were then used as predictors for the other variables. We then describe the scientists’ own perceptions of the hazards and risk from their volcano. This section discusses the variability in the volcanoes represented in the survey in terms of perceived hazard, and is therefore important in framing the subsequent sections. The next section presents results concerning scientists’ own perceptions of the volcanic activity and risk. It discusses patterns within the data that seem to affect the perceived risk level. Finally, we discuss the scientists’ views about local people’s perception of risk, and the factors that affect this. This section also models some of the scientists’ attitudes concerning warnings, trust and precaution.

Demography

Of respondents to the survey (total 95), 69% were male, 62% had doctoral degrees, 85% had worked at volcano observatories as scientists, 7% as directors; 41% had lived in their current community for more than 20 years and 56% lived within 100 km of their specified volcano. The mean age of respondents was 43, and the total age range was 23–75. In total, scientists of 27 nationalities responded. In terms of the volcanoes, 53% of respondents worked at potentially (rather than persistently or currently) active volcanoes; 95% of the volcanoes were in populated areas (within 100 km of a settlement); 25 different countries were represented and 82% of the volcanoes were subduction zone volcanoes. In total, 51% of the volcanoes had been active in the last 5 years, 64% in the last 50 years and 77% in the last 100 years. We also distinguish persistently active volcanoes as those which have been active for periods of 10 years or more at the time of the survey – 46% of the volcanoes fall into this category. A total of 67% of the volcanoes were located in developed countries, and the rest in developing countries (as defined by the International Statistical Institute, [2013]) – this is taken as a first-order measure of resourcing between nations, though we recognise that it is not a perfect measure.

Figure 1 shows the scientists’ perceptions of volcanic hazard at their chosen volcanoes. The colour of the symbol represents the perceived hazard level (on a scale from “not at all hazardous” to “extremely hazardous”), and the size represents the likelihood of a major eruption within the next 30 years. Where there were multiple responses for a single volcano, average values are shown. There was considerable variation in perceptions of hazardousness for some individual volcanoes. In terms of the types of hazard, tephra fall was the most widespread, regarded as a problem by 60% of respondents. Pyroclastic Flows (PFs) were viewed as a hazard by 46% of respondents. Gas emission was the least widely reported hazard, at 15% of volcanoes. In general, maximum expected VEI for the eruption correlated well with the estimated length of flows from individual volcanoes for lahars, debris avalanches and PFs, but not for lateral blasts, perhaps suggesting that these events have an uncertain provenance (are more difficult to anticipate). Subduction zone volcanoes were consistently associated with higher magnitude hazards.

Figure 1
figure 1

Scientists’ perceptions of volcanic hazard at their chosen volcano. Colour represents the perceived hazard level (green = low hazard, to red = high hazard), while size represents the perceived likelihood of an eruption in the next 30 years (small = unlikely, large = extremely likely).

For volcanoes with multiple respondents, there was general agreement about the likely maximum lengths for lava flows and lahars, but less agreement about PFs, blasts and debris avalanches, again suggesting that there is more uncertainty concerning more extreme phenomena. Estimates of the maximum PF distance were particularly varied, with standard deviations > 10 km for most volcanoes. This suggests that volcanologists, even those working on the same volcano, may vary in how they define a “major eruption” and in their perceptions of the likely phenomena that the volcano is capable of producing. The distance that the scientist lived from the volcano correlated negatively with the amount of information available – scientists tended to live further away from volcanoes about which there is relatively little information (ρ = −0.29 p < 0.01). Some volcanologists were not comfortable with providing numerical values, either for hazard magnitude or for probability of eruption. The variety in estimates demonstrates the uncertainty inherent in volcanic activity.

Scientists’ views about volcanic risk

This section reviews the scientists’ own views about the volcanic hazards and risk at the volcano. In general, volcanic activity was viewed as changing at a moderate level from year to year, and as being relatively difficult to understand or forecast over any timescale. Scientists who had been in their jobs for longer tended to think that volcanic activity changed less both week to week (ρ = 0.22, p < 0.05) and year to year (ρ = 0.31 p < 0.01), which might suggest a longer term perspective in scientists with greater experience. Scientists living closer to the volcano tended to view understanding the changes in the volcano’s behaviour as easier on both timescales than those who lived further away (ρ(week to week) = 0.28, p < 0.01; ρ(year to year) = 0.36, p < 0.01), again suggesting that scientists who are willing to live closer to the volcano are generally more confident about understanding it. Volcanoes that were viewed as having lower maximum VEIs also tended to be viewed as easier to understand on a week to week basis (ρ = 0.28, 0 < 0.05). This may be the result of these volcanoes being persistently active and showing similar patterns of activity over medium timescales, and more information about them being available.

With regard to impact variables, the majority of scientists felt that a temporary evacuation would be necessary when an eruption happens (M = 5.9, SD = 1.7). There was considerable variation in views concerning the effectiveness of current evacuation plans (M = 3.9, SD = 1.8). There was, however, greater confidence in current warning plans, into which scientists may have more input (M = 4.3, SD = 1.8). Older respondents were more likely to think that permanent relocation would be necessary (ρ = 0.25, p < 0.05). People who described themselves as emotionally attached to their current community were less likely to think that loss of life would be high in the event of no evacuation (ρ = −0.23, p < 0.05) and more likely to think that current warning systems and plans were effective (ρ(warnings) = 0.24, p < 0.05; ρ(evacuation) = 0.30,p < 0.01). Volcanoes about which a lot of information was available were also considered more likely to be judged to have effective warning systems (ρ = 0.21, p < 0.05). Volcanoes in developing countries were considered to have less effective warning (U = 1130, p < 0.05) and evacuation plans (U = 1123, p < 0.05). They were also more likely to be associated with a need for permanent evacuation (U = 521.5, p = 0.001).

Differences between scientists’ views and their opinions of local views

In general, scientists felt that local people viewed eruptions as less likely than they themselves did (Table 1).

Table 1 Scientists’ views about local perceptions of risk in relation to their own, for the three periods

Table 2 shows the differences between experts’ views and their views about lay perceptions concerning the volcanic activity levels and the challenges associated with forecasting. It demonstrates that scientists generally felt that local people did not have realistic perceptions of the volcanic activity.

Table 2 Percentages for different nominal categories representing comparisons between scientists’ perception of volcanic activity and their view about local people’s perceptions of activity

In general, almost half of respondents felt that local people underestimated the challenge of forecasting and understanding volcanic activity. This could be viewed as a failure to appreciate the uncertainty inherent in volcano forecasting. However, a significant number (around 30%) thought that local people viewed these activities as more challenging – and the majority of respondents clearly felt that their own perception and that of the local people were different in some way.

The differences between scientists’ views and their perceptions of local people’s views for impact variables are shown in Table 3. This demonstrates that the majority of scientists again think that there are discrepancies between their own views about eruption impacts and those of local people. In particular, a significant proportion view eruption impacts as likely to be greater than local people anticipate.

Table 3 Percentages for different nominal categories representing comparisons between scientists’ perception of local people’s views regarding the eruption impacts variables

Predicting the discrepancies

People at volcanoes with a high VEI were more likely to view local perception of risk within 3 years as lower than their own (r = −0.33, p < 0.05). Scientists at volcanoes with a high VEI also felt that local people viewed warning plans (r = −0.40, p < 0.01) and evacuation plans (r = −0.31, p < 0.05) as less effective than they did themselves. This suggests a perceived problem with local awareness of information about volcanic risk from hazardous volcanoes.

Hierarchical regression analyses were carried out for the individual impact variables. Results are shown in Table 4.

Table 4 Regression analyses for the impact variables and planning variables

Table 4 shows that the planning variables could be modelled by controlling for scientists’ views and then adding VEI as a predictor, suggesting that at volcanoes with high VEIs, local people were regarded as underestimating the value of current plans.

There were significant positive correlations between the number of years since the last eruption of the volcano and the perceived local view of ease of forecasting (r = 0.32, p < 0.05) and understanding from year to year (r = 0.35, p < 0.05): locals were viewed as overestimating the ease of understanding and forecasting volcanoes that have not been recently active. The view that locals underestimate the potential necessity of permanent evacuations was also associated with volcanoes that have not erupted for many years (r = 0.36, p < 0.05). Both of these results suggest that scientists perceive a problem with cultural memory: locals are viewed as underestimating both the scientific challenge of forecasting and the magnitude of the risk at volcanoes that have not erupted recently and of which there is therefore less awareness.

Views on trust and precaution

Trust

The results are shown in Figure 2. This shows that in general scientists viewed themselves as relatively well trusted, compared to national governments, but also rated public trust in family and in the news media as strong. Trust in scientists, national and local governments was regarded as lower at volcanoes with a higher maximum VEI (ρ(scientists) = −0.26, p < 0.05; ρ(local authorities) = −0.29, p < 0.05; ρ(national government) = −0.28, p < 0.05). The view that local people thought an eruption less likely in the next 3 or 30 years than scientists did was associated with the view that scientists are not well trusted (r (3 years) = 0.37, p < 0.001; r(30 years) = 0.29,p < 0.01). For the three year period, local authorities (r = 0.29, p < 0.01) and national governments (r = 0.27, p < 0.05) were also viewed as not well trusted. Scientists who felt emotionally attached to their communities were more likely to think that trust in scientists was high (ρ = 0.23, p < 0.05). Local community leaders were trusted more in developing countries (U = 525, p < 0.01). In general, scientists who felt that there was less discrepancy between their own view of the volcanic activity and risk, and that of local people, also tended to feel that people generally trust scientists.

Figure 2
figure 2

Responses to the trust variables.

We analysed relationships between the interpretation variables and impact variables and the trust variables. The scientists’ own view for each variable was kept constant, and the local view correlated with the other variables. Results are shown in Table 5.

Table 5 Partial correlations between impact and interpretation variables and trust in official sources, controlling for scientists’ own views

Scientists who regarded warnings and evacuation plans as effective tended to think that scientists were well trusted. The view that local people thought warning and evacuation plans were effective – and also felt they could improvise a response – was associated with higher perceived trust in scientists and national and local governments. These groups were not considered well trusted by scientists who viewed locals as underestimating the consequences of eruption (Table 5).

Precaution and false alarms

Underlying many of the concerns in this survey is the issue of precautionary decision-making: the high stakes in volcanic eruptions necessitate precaution, but the history of volcanology also implies the need for caution in avoiding false alarms. The frequency data for the statements are shown in Figure 3. Older people were more likely to agree with statement 3 (ρ = 0.21, p < 0.05) as were those who had lived in their community for a long time (ρ = 0.32, p < 0.01). People who felt attached to their current community were more likely to disagree with statement 4 (ρ = −0.22, p < 0.05). Precaution in general was viewed positively by those who had been in their profession for a long time (ρ = 0.22, p < 0.05), had lived in their current community for a long time (ρ = 0.290, p < 0.01) and were attached to their community (ρ = 0.24, p < 0.05). People from Asia and the Far East were less likely to agree with statement 3 (H(4) = 24.6, p < 0.01).

Figure 3
figure 3

Responses to the statements about warnings and precaution.

Agreement with statement 1 was less prevalent among those who felt that the volcano would produce long and damaging lahars (ρ(expected lahar distance) = −0.37, p < 0.01; ρ(maximum lahar distance) = −0.35, p < 0.01). Precaution correlated positively with estimates of a high maximum lateral blast distance (ρ = 0.32, p < 0.05), while statement 1 correlated negatively with likely blast distance (ρ = −0.33, p < 0.05) and statement 2 correlated positively with likely debris avalanche distance (ρ = 0.33, p < 0.05). In general, these results suggest that very hazardous volcanoes invite precautionary responses and less concern about false alarms.

However, scientists who regarded their volcano as highly changeable tended to support statement 3 less strongly than those at volcanoes that were less changeable (ρ = −0.27, p < 0.05). People who felt their volcano was difficult to forecast (ρ = −0.26, p < 0.05) and understand (ρ = −0.27, p < 0.05) also tended to agree less strongly with statement 2 – “it’s always better to be safe than sorry”. Difficulty in forecasting from year to year was associated with a tendency to disagree that unnecessary warnings are harmful (ρ = −0.26, p < 0.05). These results suggest that high levels of scientific uncertainty led to greater ambivalence about the use of precaution.

Table 6 shows partial correlations between statement 3 and local views about volcanic activity. This suggests that while scientists’ own views correlate negatively with this statement, their opinions of local views correlate positively. Thus, if scientists agree strongly with statement 3, they also think that local people view the volcano as changing a lot and being difficult to understand. Conversely, scientists who thought that local people underestimated the changeability and uncertainty tended to agree less strongly with statement 3.

Table 6 Partial correlations for activity variables and statement 3, controlling for scientists’ views

Statement 4 received higher agreement among those who thought that local people either under or overestimated the challenge of understanding the volcano from week to week (H(2) = 10.82, p = 0.004).

People who felt that local community leaders were well trusted also tended to agree that unnecessary warnings were harmful (ρ = 0.23, p < 0.05). People who felt that the news media were well trusted tended to disagree with this statement (ρ = −0.23, p < 0.05).

Modelling trust and precaution

Trust in scientists can be predicted by some of the discrepancy between scientists’ and perceived local opinions about the necessity of temporary evacuation and the difficulty of understanding the volcano. In general, scientists who perceive greater differences between their own views about impacts and those of the public tend to think that scientists are not well trusted (Table 7). However, scientists who think that the public view the volcano as easy to understand feel well trusted, indicating a link between the knowledge that local people are perceived to have, and the trust they are perceived to place in scientists.

Table 7 Hierarchical regression results for the trust in scientists variable

For the precaution variable, 26% of the variance can be explained by a combination of the change and understanding variables and the “how personally rewarding do you find your current profession” variable (Table 8). This analysis suggests that precaution is limited by a heightened awareness of the volcanic uncertainty (if the uncertainty is high, scientists tend to be ambivalent about issuing warnings), and that precaution is increased by a generally positive feeling about vocation.

Table 8 Results from multiple regression analysis of precaution variable

A regression analysis was carried out for the two statements relating to false alarms, but no significant predictors were found.

Discussion

Scientists’ perceptions of volcanic hazard and risk

In general, many of the volcanoes discussed in the survey were regarded as hazardous and as very likely to erupt within 300 years. Forecasting and understanding the changes in the volcanoes’ behaviour was generally seen as challenging. Volcanoes were also viewed as having high potential impacts on populations, with a considerable chance of loss of life in the event of no evacuation. Temporary evacuations would be necessary in almost all cases, while permanent evacuations would be likely in many. Concern about evacuation plans was more marked than concern about warning systems already in place. These results suggest that scientists are generally concerned about risk from the volcanoes, and view the likely impacts as very high. This is particularly the case at subduction zones, at long-dormant volcanoes and in developing countries – cases that were also associated (by correlation) with less effective planning. Persistently active volcanoes, typically erupting frequently over a period of 10 years or more, tended to have more information available, be considered less hazardous and perceived as easier to understand.

The perceived discrepancy between warning plans and evacuation plans could relate to control, since warning plans are the remit of scientists to some extent, while evacuation plans tend to be the responsibility of civil protection agencies. Thus scientists may be less aware of evacuation plans, or not trust local authorities to have them in place and carry them out effectively. However, it is noticeable that many of the concerns about plans for managing volcanic activity were felt more strongly at subduction zones and in developing countries. There was also greater discrepancy between scientists’ views and the perceived views of local people about plans at volcanoes that are highly explosive. These results are not necessarily surprising, but do suggest that there are measureable concerns about local people’s views at these volcanoes.

Scientists’ views about warnings and false alarms

The responses to the statements about precaution and false alarms were complex. This suggests that scientists view these issues as contingent, and many are ambivalent about the pros and cons of issuing warnings. However, several factors emerged:

  • Volcanoes that were considered more hazardous were associated with a tendency to issue warnings and be less concerned about false alarms;

  • Volcanoes that were changeable and difficult to understand were associated with ambivalence about issuing warnings, and more concern about false alarms;

  • Scientists who felt that local people had unrealistic views about the challenges of understanding the volcano tended to disagree that warnings should always be issued;

  • Precaution was associated first and foremost with the uncertainty surrounding the volcanic system: high uncertainty implies a reluctance to issue warnings in all cases.

These results demonstrate the complexity of the decision to issue a warning. The potential for high impact was associated with a desire to warn, but high uncertainty tended to have the opposite effect. Furthermore, those scientists who felt that the local population had inaccurate views about the challenge of understanding the volcano tended to be more ambivalent about issuing warnings. Scientists who thought that local people’s views tended towards concern about the volcano tended to be more positive about issuing warnings. The balance between issuing warnings and being concerned about “false alarms” was thus viewed as challenging and complex. There were links with perceived public perception as well as with the general uncertainty surrounding the volcanic activity.

Understanding discrepancies

In general, scientists viewed local people as underestimating the risk of eruption. They also felt that local people did not have realistic views about the changeability of the volcanic system, the difficulties associated with forecasting and understanding it, and the likely impacts of an eruption. Discrepancies between local people’s perceived view and that of scientists were also associated with lower perceived trust in scientists and governments (Table 5). This could be conceptualised as a two-way trust relationship: scientists who trust the public’s view of the risk also feel trusted. Studies have demonstrated the importance of trust in the acceptance of information about risk (e.g. Haynes et al. [2008]; Eiser et al. [2008]), but it is likely that there is a level of reciprocity: those who give information have to trust those receiving it to respond accordingly.

There were several possible reasons for the perceived discrepancies in risk perception between the scientists and their imagined populations. A significant one was the eruptive history of the volcano. Memory of past events has been identified as an important factor in risk perception (the availability bias; Tversky and Kahneman [1973]; see also Slovic et al. [1980]). It is possible that scientists unconsciously rate this concept highly in their understanding of local people’s risk perceptions. However, the prolonged study of volcanoes requires scientists to consider the experiences of past eruptions and to assess the potential future hazards from volcanoes, but the availability heuristic might lead them to overestimate the risk relative to local people who may often have less personal experience of volcanic activity (Tversky and Kahneman [1973]). Likewise, the lack of daily focus on volcanoes among the population might result in a corresponding tendency to pay less attention to any hazard, and hence perhaps to estimate risk as lower. The relationship between perceived trust in scientists and the discrepancies of risk perception does seem to suggest that scientists who feel trusted also tend to have positive views of risk perception. Nevertheless, it is a cause for concern that scientists in this survey viewed the risk from specific volcanoes as high but that they considered local people to be relatively unaware of that risk, particularly at the most explosive volcanoes.

Implications for disaster management

In general, the survey data suggest that scientists perceive a problem in risk perception on volcanoes that have high explosivity, have not erupted for a long time, and those that are not well understood (including many in developing countries). Disaster risk reduction emphasises the role of local knowledge alongside scientific knowledge in decision-making for natural hazards (e.g. Gaillard and Mercer [2013]). That scientists infer differences between their own view of the volcanic risk and that of local people is a significant result in the management of volcanic risk. It suggests the need for outreach and interaction between scientists and local people prior to and during a volcanic eruption. It may also be significant in understanding the ways in which volcanic hazard or risk assessments are framed by scientists in particular local contexts, since framing may be affected by interpretation of the likely audience (e.g. Wynne [2001]).

The question of when to issue a warning is a complex one in volcanic hazard assessment, and the data in this paper suggest that there are a number of factors that might affect this decision, including the level of uncertainty. High perceived uncertainty was associated with an ambivalence towards precaution. However, in cases where the hazard from the volcano was perceived to be high, scientists were more positive about issuing warnings. Some of the correlations suggest that concern about false alarms was greater where scientists felt that public perception of risk was less accurate. Other studies have suggested that false alarms do not necessarily have a negative impact on future responses to warnings if the reasoning behind the warning is explained to the population clearly (e.g. Mileti and Peek [2000]) or if the uncertainty surrounding the hazard event is explained (e.g. Sharma and Patt [2012]).

These findings obviously do not provide an answer to the question that has pervaded volcanology since Guadeloupe in 1976 – when to issue warnings in cases of high uncertainty. Furthermore, many scientists would argue that there is no such thing as a “false” alarm when uncertainty is high, and Hincks et al. ([2014]) found that, given the uncertainty in 1976, a Bayesian approach indicates that the evacuation was necessary even though the volcano did not erupt. What our data do suggest is that scientists’ perceptions of risk, their views about how trusted they are and how engaged they are with their local community are linked – and may also be linked with views about precaution and false alarms. Expectations about local perceptions of risk are also linked to views about these issues – scientists may be more inclined to issue warnings if they feel people are unprepared. In conditions of high uncertainty, nevertheless, there is a reluctance to issue warnings – particularly from scientists who think that the public underestimates the challenges involved in volcano forecasting. However, sustained engagement with local communities may aid the communication of warnings when volcanic activity increases.

Conclusions

This paper has presented the results from a survey of volcanologists in 2011. Our findings show that scientists perceive some discrepancies between their own perception of volcanic risk and that of local people. Within the scientific community, there is considerable variation concerning views about false alarms, while most scientists are in favour of precautionary decision-making. These views are dominantly related to the uncertainty, but also linked to perceptions about (i) whether the public fully appreciate the risk and (ii) whether the public appreciate the challenges involved in forecasting and understanding volcanic activity. It may be that a trusting public and a trusted public are linked in the minds of scientists, and this might affect the provision, or not, of warnings. It might also affect the timing and nature of warnings. An implication seems to be that risk communication might be most effective where scientists and communities are engaged with one another prior to the onset of volcanic activity.

References

  1. Alexander DE: Communicating earthquake risk to the public: the trial of the “L’Aquila Seven”. Nat Haz 2014, 72(2):1159–1173. 10.1007/s11069-014-1062-2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Bankoff G: Rendering the world unsafe:`vulnerability’as western discourse. Disasters 2001, 25(1):19–35. 10.1111/1467-7717.00159

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Bird DK, Gisladottir G, Dominey-Howes D: Public perception of jokulhlaup hazard and risk in Iceland: Implications for community education. Int J Manag Decis Making 2009, 10(3–4):164–175. 10.1504/IJMDM.2009.024985

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Bird DK, Gisladottir G, Dominey-Howes D: Volcanic risk and tourism in southern Iceland: Implications for hazard, risk and emergency response education and training. J Volcanol Geotherm Res 2010, 189(1–2):33. 10.1016/j.jvolgeores.2009.09.020

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Bostrom A: Risk perceptions: Experts vs lay people. Duke Envtl L Pol’y F 1997, 8: 101.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Brooks N: Vulnerability, risk and adaptation: A conceptual framework. 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Colton D, Covert RW: Designing and Constructing Instruments for Social Research and Evaluation. John Wiley & Sons, San Francisco; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Cutter SL: Vulnerability to environmental hazards. Progr Human Geogr 1996, 20: 529–539. 10.1177/030913259602000407

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Dash N, Gladwin H: Evacuation decision making and behavioral responses: Individual and household. Nat Hazards Rev 2007, 8(3):69–77. 10.1061/(ASCE)1527-6988(2007)8:3(69)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Dominey-Howes D, Minos-Minopoulos D: Perceptions of hazard and risk on Santorini. J Volcanol Geotherm Res 2004, 137: 285–310. 10.1016/j.jvolgeores.2004.06.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Donovan A, Oppenheimer C: Extreme volcanoes: Disaster risks and societal implications. In Extreme natural hazards: Disaster risks and societal implications. Edited by: Ismail-Zadeh A, Fucugaughi J, Kijko A, Takeuchi K, Zaliapin I. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge; 2014:29–46.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  12. Eiser JR: Public perception of risk. Report prepared for Foresight Office of Science and Technology. 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Eiser JR, Miles SM, Frewer LJ: Trust, Perceived Risk, and Attitudes toward Food Technologies. J Appl Soc Psychol 2002, 32(11):2423–2433. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2002.tb01871.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Eiser JR, Stafford T, Henneberry J, Catney P: Risk perception and trust in the context of urban brownfields. Environ Haz 2007, 7(2):150–156. 10.1016/j.envhaz.2007.05.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Eiser JR, Stafford T, Henneberry J, Catney P: “Trust me, I’m a Scientist (Not a Developer)”: Perceived Expertise and Motives as Predictors of Trust in Assessment of Risk from Contaminated Land. Risk Anal 2008, 29(2):288–297. 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2008.01131.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Fischhoff B, Slovic P, Lichtenstein S: Lay foibles and expert fables in judgments about risk. Am Stat 1982, 36(3b):240–255. 10.1080/00031305.1982.10482845

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Frewer LJ, Scholderer J, Bredahl L: Communicating about the risks and benefits of genetically modified foods: the mediating role of trust. Risk Anal 2003, 23: 1117–1133. 10.1111/j.0272-4332.2003.00385.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Gaillard J-C: Alternative paradigms of volcanic risk perception: The case of Mt Pinatubo in the Philippines. J Volcanol Geotherm Res 2008, 172(3–4):315. 10.1016/j.jvolgeores.2007.12.036

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Gaillard JC: Vulnerability, capacity and resilience: perspectives for climate and development policy. J Int Dev 2010, 22(2):218–232. 10.1002/jid.1675

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Gaillard JC, Mercer J: From knowledge to action Bridging gaps in disaster risk reduction. Progr Human Geogr 2013, 37(1):93–114. 10.1177/0309132512446717

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Green RG, Murphy KD, Snyder SM: Should demographics be placed at the end or at the beginning of mailed questionnaires? An empirical answer to a persistent methodological question. Social Work Res 2000, 24: 237–241. 10.1093/swr/24.4.237

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Gregg CE, Houghton BF, Johnston DM, Paton D, Swanson DA: The perception of volcanic risk in Kona communities from Mauna Loa and Hualalai volcanoes, Hawai'i. J Volcanol Geotherm Res 2004, 130: 179–196. 10.1016/S0377-0273(03)00288-9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Gregg CE, Houghton BF, Paton D, Swanson DA, Lachman R, Bonk WJ: Hawaiian cultural influences on support for lava flow hazard mitigation measures during the January 1960 eruption of Kilauea volcano, Kapoho, Hawaii. J Volcanol Geotherm Res 2008, 172(3–4):300. 10.1016/j.jvolgeores.2007.12.025

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Hansen J, Holm L, Frewer LJ, Robinson P, Sandøe P: Beyond the knowledge deficit: recent research into lay and expert attitudes to food risks. Appetite 2003, 41(2):111–121. 10.1016/S0195-6663(03)00079-5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Haynes K, Barclay J, Pidgeon N: The issue of trust and its influence on risk communication during a volcanic crisis. Bull Volcanol 2007, 70(5):605–621. 10.1007/s00445-007-0156-z

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Haynes K, Barclay J, Pidgeon N: Whose reality counts? Factors affecting the perception of volcanic risk. J Volcanol Geotherm Res 2008, 172(3–4):259. 10.1016/j.jvolgeores.2007.12.012

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Hewitt K: The idea of calamity in a technocratic age. In Interpretation of Calamity from the viewpoint of human ecology. Edited by: Hewitt K. Allen & Unwin Inc, Boston; 1983:3–32.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Hewitt K: Safe place or `catastrophic society’? Perspectives on hazards and disasters in Canada. Can Geogr 2000, 44(4):325–341. 10.1111/j.1541-0064.2000.tb00716.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Hewitt K: Environmental disasters in social context: toward a preventive and precautionary approach. Nat Haz 2013, 66(1):3–14. 10.1007/s11069-012-0205-6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Hincks T, Komorowski J-C, Sparks RSJ, Aspinall W: Retrospective analysis of uncertain eruption precursors at La Soufriere volcano, Guadeloupe, 1975–1977: volcanic hazard assessment using a Bayesian Belief Network approach. J Appl Volc 2014, 3: 3. 10.1186/2191-5040-3-3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. International Statistical Institute (2013) ISI Developing Countries. Available at . Accessed Aug 2014, [http://www.isi-web.org/component/content/article/5-root/root/577-developing2012]

  32. Kahneman D, Slovic P, Tversky A: Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge; 1982.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  33. Krebs JR: Risk, uncertainty and regulation. Phil Trans R Soc A 2011, 369(1956):4842–4852. 10.1098/rsta.2011.0174

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Likert R: A technique for the measurement of attitudes. Archives of psychology. 1932.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Mercer J, Kelman I, Taranis L, Suchet-Pearson S: Framework for integrating indigenous and scientific knowledge for disaster risk reduction. Disasters 2010, 34(1):214–239. 10.1111/j.1467-7717.2009.01126.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Mileti DS, O'Brien PW: Warnings during disaster: Normalizing communicated risk. Soc Probs 1992, 39: 40. 10.2307/3096912

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Mileti DS, Peek L: The social psychology of public response to warnings of a nuclear power plant accident. J Haz Mat 2000, 75(2):181–194. 10.1016/S0304-3894(00)00179-5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Mileti DS, Sorensen JH: Communication of emergency public warnings: A social science perspective and state-of-the-art assessment. Oak Ridge National Lab, TN (USA); 1990.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  39. Paton D, Smith L, Daly M, Johnston D: Risk perception and volcanic hazard mitigation: Individual and social perspectives. J Volcanol Geotherm Res 2008, 172(3–4):179. 10.1016/j.jvolgeores.2007.12.026

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Paton D, Sagala S, Okada N, Jang LJ, Burgelt PT, Gregg CE: Making sense of natural hazard mitigation: Personal, social and cultural influences. Environ Haz 2010, 9(2):183–196. 10.3763/ehaz.2010.0039

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Pidgeon N, Fischhoff B: The role of social and decision sciences in communicating uncertain climate risks. Nat Clim Change 2011, 1(1):35–41. 10.1038/nclimate1080

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Pidgeon N, Kasperson RE, Slovic P: The social amplification of risk. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge; 2003.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  43. Poortinga W, Pidgeon NF: Exploring the dimensionality of trust in risk regulation. Risk Anal 2003, 23(5):961–972. 10.1111/1539-6924.00373

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Poortinga W, Bickerstaff K, Langford I, Niewöhner J, Pidgeon N: The British 2001 foot and mouth crisis: a comparative study of public risk perceptions, trust and beliefs about government policy in two communities. J Risk Res 2004, 7(1):73–90. 10.1080/1366987042000151205

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Renn O, Levine D: Credibility and trust in risk communication. In Communicating Risks to the Public. Edited by: Stallen P, Kasperson R. Springer, Netherlands; 1991:175–217. 10.1007/978-94-009-1952-5_10

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  46. Rowe G, Wright G: Differences in Expert and Lay Judgments of Risk: Myth or Reality? Risk Anal 2001, 21(2):341–356. 10.1111/0272-4332.212116

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Savadori L, Savio S, Nicotra E, Rumiati R, Finucane M, Slovic P: Expert and Public Perception of Risk from Biotechnology. Risk Anal 2004, 24(5):1289–1299. 10.1111/j.0272-4332.2004.00526.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Sharma U, Patt A: Disaster warning response: the effects of different types of personal experience. Nat Haz 2012, 60(2):409–423. 10.1007/s11069-011-0023-2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Sjoberg L: Factors in risk perception. Risk Anal 2000, 20: 1–11. 10.1111/0272-4332.00001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Slovic P: Trust, emotion, sex, politics, and science: Surveying the risk-assessment battlefield. Risk Anal 1999, 19(4):689–701.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Slovic P: The perception of risk. Earthscan, London; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Slovic P, Fischhoff B, Lichtenstein S: Societal risk assessment. Edited by: Schwing RC, Albers WA Jr. Springer, US; 1980:181–216. 10.1007/978-1-4899-0445-4_9

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  53. Slovic P, Finucane ML, Peters E, MacGregor DG: Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Anal 2004, 24(2):311–322. 10.1111/j.0272-4332.2004.00433.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Tazieff H: La Soufriere, volcanology and forecasting. Nature 1977, 269: 96–97. 10.1038/269096a0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Teclaw R, Price MC, Osatuke K: Demographic Question Placement: Effect on Item Response Rates and Means of a Veterans Health Administration Survey. J Bus Psychol 2012, 27(3):281–290. 10.1007/s10869-011-9249-y

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Tversky A, Kahneman D: Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cogn Psychol 1973, 5(2):207–232. 10.1016/0010-0285(73)90033-9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Tversky A, Kahneman D: Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Science 1974, 185(4157):1124–1131. 10.1126/science.185.4157.1124

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Wachinger G, Renn O, Begg C, Kohlicke C: The risk perception paradox: Implications for governance and communication of natural hazards. Risk Anal 2013, 33(6):1049–1065. 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2012.01942.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Wadge G, Voight B, Sparks RSJ, Cole PD, Loughlin SC, Robertson R: An overview of the eruption of Soufriere Hills Volcano, Montserrat from 2000 to 2010. In The eruption of the Soufriere Hills Volcano, Montserrat, from 2000–2010 Edited by: Wadge G, Robertson R, Voight B. 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  60. White GF: Human adjustment to floods. University of Chicago, Chicago; 1945.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Wisner B, Blaikie P, Cannon T, Davis I: At risk Natural hazards people's vulnerability and disasters. Routledge, London; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Wisner B, Gaillard JC, Kelman I: Handbook of hazards and disaster risk reduction. Routledge, London; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Wynne B: A reflexive view of the expert-lay knowledge divide. In Risk, environment and modernity: Towards a new ecology. Edited by: Lash S, Szerszynski B, Wynne B. Sage, London; 1996:44–83.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Wynne B: Creating public alienation: expert cultures of risk and ethics on GMOs. Sci Cult 2001, 10(4):445–481. 10.1080/09505430120093586

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Wynne B: Risk and Environment as Legitimatory Discourses of Technology: Reflexivity Inside Out? Curr Sociol 2002, 50(3):459–477. 10.1177/0011392102050003010

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was funded by VOLDIES (Advanced European Research Council Grant to R.S.J. Sparks). The authors would like to thank the respondents and the World Organisation of Volcano Observatories for their help in disseminating and testing the survey.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Amy Donovan.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

All three authors were involved in survey design. AD analysed the results and wrote the paper with support from JRE and RSJS. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Authors’ original file for figure 3

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits use, duplication, adaptation, distribution, and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Donovan, A., Eiser, J.R. & Sparks, R.S.J. Scientists’ views about lay perceptions of volcanic hazard and risk. J Appl. Volcanol. 3, 15 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13617-014-0015-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13617-014-0015-5

Keywords